Discovery-based learning

10
 m

Design and plan learning activities and/or programmes of study

How students learn, both generally and within their subject/disciplinary area(s)

Use evidence-informed approaches and the outcomes from research, scholarship and continuing professional development

What can I do?

Impact
4
Quality
4
  • Give students feedback, prompts, or worked examples when using discovery-based tasks
  • Use inquiry or discovery activities after teaching key knowledge, not before
  • Avoid using unassisted discovery — it doesn’t help and may hurt learning

What is this about?

Discovery-based learning is when students figure out ideas for themselves instead of being told the answers. They might run experiments, solve puzzles, or explore problems without step-by-step instructions. In higher education, it’s used in labs, problem-solving, and project-based learning. But students need help — too little guidance can overwhelm them or lead them in the wrong direction. When well supported, discovery learning builds deeper understanding and engagement.

What's the evidence say?

Unassisted discovery learning has a small but clear negative effect on student learning ➖➖➖ (Alfieri et al., 2011). In contrast, enhanced or guided discovery — with tools like scaffolds, elicited explanations, or feedback — leads to better learning outcomes than traditional instruction ➕➕➕. Inquiry learning with guidance is especially powerful in science and maths, improving not just test results (d = 0.50), but how well students complete the task (d = 0.71) ➕➕➕➕ (Lazonder & Harmsen, 2016). The type of guidance matters: scaffolds, prompts, and worked examples are most effective. The evidence also shows that younger or less experienced learners benefit most from more specific guidance.

What's the underlying theory?

Discovery learning draws on constructivist theories from Piaget and Bruner, which say students learn best when they build their own understanding. But theories like Cognitive Load Theory say too much freedom can overload the brain, especially if students are new to the topic. That’s why Scaffolding Theory recommends giving just the right amount of help. Guided discovery gives students freedom to explore while still supporting them to succeed — making learning both active and achievable.

Where does the evidence come from?

This summary is based on two high-quality meta-analyses. Alfieri et al. (2011) reviewed 164 studies and found clear distinctions between unassisted and guided discovery. Lazonder and Harmsen (2016) analysed 72 studies of inquiry-based learning in science and maths. Both used robust statistical methods, tested key moderators (like age and type of guidance), and were rated high quality (++++) using GRADE.

References

Alfieri, L., Brooks, P. J., Aldrich, N. J., & Tenenbaum, H. R. (2011). Does discovery-based instruction enhance learning? Journal of Educational Psychology, 103(1), 1–18. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0021017

Lazonder, A. W., & Harmsen, R. (2016). Meta-analysis of inquiry-based learning: Effects of guidance. Review of Educational Research, 86(3), 681–718. https://doi.org/10.3102/0034654315627366

Additional Resources